nep-exp New Economics Papers
on Experimental Economics
Issue of 2009‒01‒24
eight papers chosen by
Daniel Houser
George Mason University

  1. Reference Points and Effort Provision By Abeler, Johannes; Falk, Armin; Goette, Lorenz; Huffman, David
  2. Does competition enhance performance or cheating? A laboratory experiment By Christiane Schwieren; Doris Weichselbaumer
  3. Voluntary participation and cooperation in a collective-good game. By Kene Boun My; Benoît Chalvignac
  4. Risk Tolerance, Self-Interest, and Social Preferences By Lucy F. Ackert; Ann B. Gillette; Jorge Martinez-Vazquez; Mark Rider
  5. Procedural Satisfaction Matters - Procedural Fairness does not: An Experiment Studying the Effects of Procedural Judgments on Outcome Acceptance By Vanessa Mertins
  6. Pricing Strategies Under Emissions Trading: An Experimental Analysis By Mandell, Svante; Wråke, Markus; Myers, Erica C.; Burtraw, Dallas; Holt, Charles A.
  7. The effects of Procedures on Social Interaction: A Literature Review By Vanessa Mertins
  8. Clean Evidence on Face-to-Face: Why Experimental Economics is of Interest to Regional Economists By Björn Frank

  1. By: Abeler, Johannes (University of Nottingham); Falk, Armin (University of Bonn); Goette, Lorenz (University of Geneva); Huffman, David (Swarthmore College)
    Abstract: A key open question for theories of reference-dependent preferences is what determines the reference point. One candidate is expectations: what people expect could affect how they feel about what actually occurs. In a real-effort experiment, we manipulate the rational expectations of subjects and check whether this manipulation influences their effort provision. We find that effort provision is significantly different between treatments in the way predicted by models of expectation-based reference-dependent preferences: if expectations are high, subjects work longer and earn more money than if expectations are low.
    Keywords: reference points, expectations, loss aversion, risk aversion, disappointment, experiment
    JEL: C91 D01 D84 J22
    Date: 2009–01
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:iza:izadps:dp3939&r=exp
  2. By: Christiane Schwieren (University of Heidelberg); Doris Weichselbaumer
    Abstract: In this paper we experimentally test whether competing for a desired reward does not only affect individuals’ performance, but also their tendency to cheat. Recent doping scandals in sports as well as forgery and plagiarism scandals in academia have been partially explained by „competitive pressures“, which suggests a link between competition and cheating. In our experiment subjects conduct a task where they have the possibility to make use of illegitimate tools to better their results. We find that women react much stronger to competitive pressure by increasing their cheating activity while there is no overall sex difference in cheating. However, the effect of competition on women’s cheating behavior is entirely due to the fact that women, on average, are doing worse with respect to the assigned task. Indeed we find that it is the ability of an individual to conduct a particular task and not sex that crucially affects the reaction to competition. Poor performers significantly increase their cheating behavior under competition which may be a face-saving strategy or an attempt to retain a chance of winning.
    Keywords: competition, tournament, piece rate, cheating, experiment
    JEL: C91 J24 J31 M52
    Date: 2008–12
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:jku:nrnwps:2008_05&r=exp
  3. By: Kene Boun My; Benoît Chalvignac
    Abstract: We study the effect of voluntary participation in the context of a collective-good experiment. We investigate whether the freedom to participate in the game or not increases contribution levels and enhances their evolution. The analysis of two voluntary participation treatments supports a positive effect of an attractive exit option on both contribution levels and their sustainability. We conclude that the voluntary contribution mechanism can provide sustainable cooperation levels and that the usually observed decay of average contribution levels can be counteracted by voluntary participation in the game..
    Keywords: Collective Goods; Cooperation; Voluntary participation ; Laboratory experiments.
    JEL: H41 C92
    Date: 2009
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:ulp:sbbeta:2009-01&r=exp
  4. By: Lucy F. Ackert; Ann B. Gillette; Jorge Martinez-Vazquez; Mark Rider
    Abstract: We use an experimental method to investigate whether systematic relationships exist across distinct aspects of individual preferences: risk aversion in monetary outcomes, altruism in a twoperson context, and social preferences in a larger group context. Individual preferences across these three contexts are measured, and there is no possibility for risk sharing, wealth effects, or updating expectations of the population choices. We find that social preferences are related to demographic variables, including years of education, gender, and age. Perhaps most importantly, self allocation in a two-person dictator game is related to social preferences in a group context. Participants who are more generous in a dictator game are more likely to vote against their selfinterest in a group decision-making task which we interpret to be expressions of social preferences.
    JEL: C91 C92 D63 H21
    Date: 2009–01
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:exc:wpaper:2009-04&r=exp
  5. By: Vanessa Mertins (Institute for Labour Law and Industrial Relations in the EC, University of Trier)
    Abstract: By reporting data from a laboratory experiment, we provide clear evidence that people value procedures apart from their effects on consequences. We implement a game with one proposer who has distributive power over a pie and four responders who can invest in resistance against the proposer's demand. The proposer is appointed by the use of one of two feasible appointment procedures. We elicit participants' preferences and fairness evaluations over both procedures and study whether responders' resistance against various demands are affected by their procedural judgments. Although the fair process effect, describing the finding that people are more likely to accept outcomes when they feel that they are made via fair procedures, is said to be exceedingly robust, we do not find support for any significant behavioral dfferences according to people's fairness evaluations. In contrast, we show that procedural satisfaction matters. Surprisingly, responders whose procedural preferences are satiffed offer significantly more resistance than those whose procedural preferences are violated.
    Keywords: experiment, fair process effect, frustration effect, procedural fairness, procedural preferences, resistance, threshold public good
    JEL: C72 C91 J52 D23
    Date: 2008–12
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:iaa:wpaper:200807&r=exp
  6. By: Mandell, Svante (vti - Swedish National Road and Transport Research Institute); Wråke, Markus (IVL); Myers, Erica C. (RFF - Resources for the future); Burtraw, Dallas (RFF - Resources for the future); Holt, Charles A. (University of Virginia - Department of Economics)
    Abstract: An important feature in the design of an emissions trading program is how emissions allowances are initially distributed into the market. In a competitive market the choice between an auction and free allocation should, according to economic theory, not have any influence on firms’ production choices nor on consumer prices. However, many observers expect the method of allocation to affect product prices. This paper reports on the use of experimental methods to investigate behavior with respect to how prices will be determined under a cap-and-trade program. Participants initially display a variety of pricing strategies. However, given a simple economic setting in which earnings depend on this behavior, we find that subjects learn to consider the value of allowances and overall behavior moves toward that predicted by economic theory.
    Keywords: carbon dioxide; climate change; emissions trading; distributional effects; electricity; allocation; auctions
    JEL: C91 D44
    Date: 2009–01–13
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:hhs:vtiwps:2009_001&r=exp
  7. By: Vanessa Mertins (Institute for Labour Law and Industrial Relations in the EC, University of Trier)
    Abstract: While economists have neglected procedures for a long time, other social scientists early established a substantial research program. By now, there exists a large gap between a sheer bulk of empirical, experimental, and theoretical studies by non-economists and the fact that there is hardly any economic research on procedures. We argue that due to clear evidence for procedures in uencing human decision-making, economists can not remain silent about procedural aspects of strategic interactions any longer. There is an important research agenda to be developed. This survey article is intended to discuss an important approach by which the standard economic model, which is based on consequentialist preferences, needs to be enriched: not only outcomes shape human behavior but also the way in which decisions are taken. Behavioral economics may serve as an important link. Its aim is to integrate insights of cognitive and social psychologists as well as experimental economists with neoclassical economic theory. We argue that experimental economics should increase its efforts to identify procedural effects and that these experiments should be more incorporated in the theoretical literature as part of an ongoing dialogue between theorists and experimentalists. Among procedural aspects, procedural fairness suggests itself to become an integrative part. To highlight the need for rethinking the standard economic approach we review social science literature on procedural effects, with a special focus on experimental economics and inspired theory-building.
    Date: 2008–12
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:iaa:wpaper:200806&r=exp
  8. By: Björn Frank (University Kassel, Nora-Platiel-Str. 4, 34109 Kassel, Germany)
    Abstract: The notion of face-to-face contacts has recently become very popular in regional economics and in economic geography. This is the most obvious way to explain why firms still locate in proximity to others after the "death of distance", i.e., the shrinking costs for transportation, especially transportation of messages' pure information content. While this is intuitive, controlled laboratory experiments provide much more direct and reliable evidence on the importance of face-to-face contacts. They tackle the question what personal contacts are good for, and in which cases their effects are negligible. To the best of my knowledge, regional economists and geographers are not aware of this new and developing string of literature; it is the purpose of this paper to survey and to organize the relevant experimental research with a special focus on its importance for regional economics. However, the paper might also serve to alert more experimentalists to the importance of their work for current regional science, of which they seem not to be aware either.
    Keywords: Cooperation, death of distance, face-to-face, localized spillovers, trust
    JEL: C90 D83 R19
    Date: 2009
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:mar:magkse:200904&r=exp

This nep-exp issue is ©2009 by Daniel Houser. It is provided as is without any express or implied warranty. It may be freely redistributed in whole or in part for any purpose. If distributed in part, please include this notice.
General information on the NEP project can be found at http://nep.repec.org. For comments please write to the director of NEP, Marco Novarese at <director@nep.repec.org>. Put “NEP” in the subject, otherwise your mail may be rejected.
NEP’s infrastructure is sponsored by the School of Economics and Finance of Massey University in New Zealand.