nep-exp New Economics Papers
on Experimental Economics
Issue of 2007‒08‒14
six papers chosen by
Daniel Houser
George Mason University

  1. Microfinance Games By Xavier Gine; Pamela Jakiela; Dean Karlan; Jonathan Morduch
  2. Social Identity and Preferences By Daniel J. Benjamin; James J. Choi; A. Joshua Strickland
  3. Individual Risk Attitudes: New Evidence from a Large, Representative, Experimentally-Validated Survey By Thomas Dohmen; Armin Falk; David Huffman; Uwe Sunde; Juergen Schupp; Gert Wagner
  4. Do You Know That I Am Biased? An Experiment By Sandra Ludwig; Julia Nafziger
  5. The control of game form recognition in experiments: Understanding dominant strategy failures in a simple two person “Guessing” game By Chou, Eileen; McConnell, Margaret; Nagel, Rosemarie; Plott, Charles R.
  6. Deterministic and Stochastic Prisoner's Dilemma Games: Experiments in Interdependent Security By Howard Kunreuther; Gabriel Silvasi; Eric T. Bradlow; Dylan Small

  1. By: Xavier Gine (World Bank); Pamela Jakiela (University of California, Berkeley); Dean Karlan (Yale University); Jonathan Morduch (New York University)
    Abstract: Microfinance has been heralded as an effective way to address imperfections in credit markets. From a theoretical perspective, however, the success of microfinance contracts has puzzling elements. In particular, the group-based mechanisms often employed are vulnerable to free-riding and collusion, although they can also reduce moral hazard and improve selection. We created an experimental economics laboratory in a large urban market in Lima, Peru and over seven months conducted eleven different games that allow us to unpack microfinance mechanisms in a systematic way. We find that risk-taking broadly conforms to predicted patterns, but that behavior is safer than optimal. The results help to explain why pioneering microfinance institutions have been moving away from group-based contracts. The work also provides an example of how to use framed field experiments as a methodological bridge between laboratory and field experiments.
    Keywords: microfinance, group lending, information asymmetries, contract theory, experimental economics
    JEL: O12 D92 D10 D21 D82 C93
    Date: 2006–09
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:feb:wpaper:2102&r=exp
  2. By: Daniel J. Benjamin; James J. Choi; A. Joshua Strickland
    Abstract: In two laboratory experiments, we examine whether norms associated with one's social identity affect time and risk preferences. When we make ethnic identity salient to Asian-American subjects, they make more patient choices. When we make race salient to black subjects, non-immigrant blacks (but not immigrant blacks) make more risk-averse choices. Making gender identity salient causes choices to conform to gender norms the subject believes are relatively more common. Our results provide evidence that identity effects play a role in shaping U.S. demographic patterns in economic behaviors and outcomes.
    JEL: C91 Z10
    Date: 2007–08
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:nbr:nberwo:13309&r=exp
  3. By: Thomas Dohmen (Institute for the Study of Labor, IZA); Armin Falk (IZA and University of Bonn); David Huffman (Institute for the Study of Labor, IZA); Uwe Sunde (IZA and University of Bonn); Juergen Schupp (German Institute for Economic Research, DIW); Gert Wagner (Berlin University of Technology, DIW, and Cornell University)
    Abstract: This paper presents new evidence on the distribution of risk attitudes in the population, using a novel set of survey questions and a representative sample of roughly 22,000 individuals living in Germany. Using a question that asks about willingness to take risks in general, on an 11-point scale, we find evidence of heterogeneity across individuals, and show that willingness to take risks is negatively related to age and being female, and positively related to height and parental education. We test the behavioral relevance of this survey measure by conducting a complementary field experiment, based on a representative sample of 450 subjects, and find that the general risk question is a good predictor of actual risk-taking behavior. We then use a more standard lottery question to measure risk preferences in our sample of 22,000, and find similar results regarding heterogeneity and determinants of risk preferences, compared to the general risk question. The lottery question also makes it possible to estimate the coefficient of relative risk aversion for each individual in the sample. Using five questions about willingness to take risks in specific domains - car driving, financial matters, sports and leisure, career, and health - the paper also studies the impact of context on risk attitudes, finding a strong but imperfect correlation across contexts. Using data on a collection of risky behaviors from different contexts, including traffic offenses, portfolio choice, smoking, occupational choice, participation in sports, and migration, the paper compares the predictive power of all of the risk measures. Strikingly, the general risk question predicts all behaviors whereas the standard lottery measure does not. The best predictor for any specific behavior is typically the corresponding context-specific measure.
    Keywords: Risk Preferences, Experimental Validation, Field Experiment, SOEP, Gender Differences, Context, Age, Height, Subjective Well-Being, Migration, Occupational Choice, Health
    JEL: D0 D1 D80 D81 C91 C93 J16 J24 J61 I1
    Date: 2005–11
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:feb:wpaper:2096&r=exp
  4. By: Sandra Ludwig; Julia Nafziger
    Abstract: This experiment explores whether individuals know that other people are biased. We confirm that overestimation of abilities is a pervasive problem, but observe that most people are not aware of it, i.e. they think others are unbiased. We investigate several explanations for this result. As a first one, we discuss a possible unfamiliarity with the task and the subjects' inability to distinguish between random mistakes and a real bias.  Second, we show how the relation between a subject's belief about others and his belief about himself might be driven by a false consensus effect or self-correction mechanism. Third, we identify a self-serving bias when comparing how a subject evaluates his own and other people's biases.
    Keywords: Bias, Overconfidence, Beliefs, Experimental Economics, Self-Serving Bias
    JEL: D83 C91 D01
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:bon:bonedp:bgse11_2007&r=exp
  5. By: Chou, Eileen; McConnell, Margaret; Nagel, Rosemarie; Plott, Charles R.
    Date: 2007–08
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:clt:sswopa:1274&r=exp
  6. By: Howard Kunreuther; Gabriel Silvasi; Eric T. Bradlow; Dylan Small
    Abstract: This paper examines experiments on interdependent security prisoner's dilemma games with repeated play. By utilizing a Bayesian hierarchical model, we examine how subjects make investment decisions as a function of their previous experience and their treatment condition. Our main findings are that individuals have differing underlying propensities to invest that vary across time, are affected by both the stochastic nature of the game and even more so by an individual's ability to learn about his or her counterpart's choices. Implications for individual decisions and the likely play of a person's counterpart are discussed in detail.
    JEL: C11 C12 C22 C23 C73 C91
    Date: 2007–08
    URL: http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:nbr:nberte:0341&r=exp

This nep-exp issue is ©2007 by Daniel Houser. It is provided as is without any express or implied warranty. It may be freely redistributed in whole or in part for any purpose. If distributed in part, please include this notice.
General information on the NEP project can be found at http://nep.repec.org. For comments please write to the director of NEP, Marco Novarese at <director@nep.repec.org>. Put “NEP” in the subject, otherwise your mail may be rejected.
NEP’s infrastructure is sponsored by the School of Economics and Finance of Massey University in New Zealand.